|
Searching for the truth (Khmer version) July & August,
Number 31 & 32, 2002
A Magazine of the Documentation Center of
Cambodia
AGENTS OF DEATH: EXPLAINING THE CAMBODIAN GENOCIDE
IN TERMS OF PSYCHOSOCIAL DISSONANCE
Alex Hinton
Department of Anthropology
Rutgers University
ahinton@andromeda.rutgers.edu
Many people have asked me: how could the Cambodian peasant,
whom we had always regarded as gentle and charming and smiling and civilized
turn into the kind of tough and grim and even brutal revolutionary who entered
Phnom Penh on April 17? I have no easy answers. [Schanberg 1975:29]
The existence of dissonance, being psychologically
uncomfortable, will motivate the person to try to reduce the dissonance and
achieve consonance. [Festinger 1957:3]
People often used to characterize Cambodia as a "gentle land" inhabited
by nonviolent Buddhists who were always courteous, friendly, and ready with a
smile. Beginning in the late 1960's, however, the country was rocked by
socioeconomic unrest, civil war, intensive U.S. bombing, and, finally, social
revolution. While around six hundred thousand of Cambodia's eight million
inhabitants perished during these years, up to a million and a half people later
died from disease, starvation, overwork, and execution during Democratic
Kampuchea (1975-1979).[i] Survivor accounts are
replete with stories of how the Khmer Rouge shot, bludgeoned, stabbed, and
tortured legions of their own country people. This type of violence demands the
attention of scholars. How could the seemingly "gentle" Cambodians come to
commit such genocidal acts?
While other disciplines have addressed this challenging issue,
anthropology has been remarkably silent on the topic of large-scale genocide, an
omission that is particularly striking because anthropologists have demonstrated
the ability to productively explain the roots of violence in other,
non-genocidal contexts.[ii] To help to redress this
deficiency, this essay will provide a psychosocial explanation of how people
come to commit acts of genocide. Drawing on the psychological concept of
cognitive dissonance (Festinger 1957), modified and enhanced by anthropological
notions of the self, cultural models, and emotion, I will analyze the events
that took place in Cambodia in terms of what I call "psychosocial dissonance"
(PSD). By doing so, I intend to provide a starting point for anthropological
debate on large-scale genocide, to delineate an explanation for how people come
to commit genocidal atrocities, to develop a general explanatory model that can
be applied to other genocides, and to explain the psychosocial origins of the
Cambodian genocide.
The first section of this essay briefly outlines the theory of cognitive
dissonance and then illustrates how it can be developed by anthropological
concepts into a model of "psychosocial dissonance." I then describe two salient
cultural models that existed prior to Democratic Kampuchea, the "gentle ethic"
and the "violent ethic." The third section shows how these models came into
conflict when the Khmer Rouge attempted to radically transform Cambodian
society, thus creating a situation of PSD. I then illustrate the steps the DK
regime took to help reduce the PSD of its "agents of death." The fourth section
illustrates how, in part by drawing on this "state level response," Khmer Rouge
cadre made a series of cognitive moves to reduce their PSD -- i.e., the
"individual level response." I conclude by discussing the implications of PSD
for the anthropological study of large-scale genocide.
A Psychosocial Model of Cognitive Dissonance
Cognitive Dissonance
Leon Fstinger's (1957) original formulation of "cognitive dissonance"
(CD) theory asserts that if a person holds two conflicting cognitions, she or he
will be motivated to reduce the resulting state of psychological discomfort in a
manner similar to drive reduction. Upon hearing a report that cigarette smoking
is bad for their health, for example, many smokers will likely be motivated to
reduce the resulting psychological discomfort/dissonance by: changing cognitions
to make them more compatible (e.g., dismissing the research out of hand);
circumspectly adding new cognitions that bridge the gap between the cognitive
elements (e.g., finding information that indicates smoking is less dangerous
than driving a car); or changing her or his behavior (e.g., stopping smoking).
The stronger the "magnitude" of dissonance, the more a person will be motivated
to reduce it.
This theory has generated a great deal of research, much of which
indicates that cognitive dissonance is greatest when an individual has a
behavioral commitment to one or both of the conflicting cognitions (Aronson
1992). Such experimental findings and a growing dissatisfaction with the
vagueness of the original formulation of dissonance theory have led Elliot
Aronson to assert that cognitive dissonance "is clearest and greatest when it
involves not just any two cognitions but, rather, a cognition about the self and
a piece of our behavior that violates that self-concept" (1992:305). Dissonance
therefore arises in situations in which a person is confronted with behavioral
expectations that conflict with this concept of the self.
While the theory of cognitive dissonance and its later reformulations
provide a great deal of insight into human thought and behavior, this concept is
nevertheless predicated on some of the biases of Western psychology. From an
anthropological perspective, this cognitive dissonance research is problematical
in its: 1) treatment of the relationship between culture and individual
variation; 2) lack of an adequate theory of motivation; 3) insufficient
attention to contextual complexity; 4) transcendent conceptualization of the
self; and 5) disregard of emotion. As opposed to simply dismissing the theory,
however, we can revise it in accordance with theoretical developments about
cultural models, the self, and emotion.
Anthropological Contributions: Cultural Models and the
Self
Problem #1 - Culture: While Festinger (1957) included "cultural
mores" as a potential source of dissonance in his original formulation of CD
theory, culture was never treated in a very sophisticated manner in later
research. Recent developments in cognitive anthropology, however, have made it
possible to bridge the gap between culture and cognition through the concept of
cognitive schema, or knowledge structures through which people interpret stimuli
and determine appropriate behavioral responses (D'Andrade 1995).
The key move cognitive anthropologists have made is to illustrate that a
large number of these cognitive schemas are culturally shared. While no exact
correspondence exists between such cultural models and the way they are
internalized by different social actors, individual variability is somewhat
constrained by language and shared social experience (D'Andrade and Strauss
1992). When going to a restaurant, for example, the expectations and behavior of
most members of our society are largely mediated by a "restaurant script" (e.g.,
entering, ordering, eating, and exiting; see Shank and Abelson 1977) that they
have learned through childhood observations, personal experience, dramatizations
on TV, books, etc. One of the strengths of cultural models theory lies in its
ability to illustrate the relationship between such personal knowledge
structures and their social analogs (Shore 1996). By connecting cognition and
culture, the concept of cultural models thus provides a means for culture to be
actively incorporated into CD theory.
Problem #2 - Motivation: Dissonance theory lacks an adequate
concept of motivation (Kunda 1990). Festinger originally asserted that people
would be motivated to decrease dissonance in a manner similar to the way they
pursue drive reduction. While Aronson's introduction of the self-concept adds
greater specificity to this "black box," the processes underlying CD motivation
remain vague. The concept of motivation delineated in cultural models research
can therefore help bring much needed specificity to CD theory.
Cognitive anthropologists have argued that, as opposed to just being a
response to an internal drive stimulus, motivation can be explained more
complexly in terms of schemas since they often establish goals that instigate
action (D'Andrade and Strauss 1992). Thus, within the restaurant script,
dissatisfaction with service may motivate a person to complain to the manager.
It is important to recognize the hierarchical arrangement of such schemas. In
this case, the "unsatisfactory service" schema is one component of the
higher-level restaurant script. While this lower-level schema may have links to
other models (e.g., to a "shopping" or an "auto-repair" schema), interpretations
tend to be passed on to "the topmost level of interpretation which is typically
linked to the actions by which the organism operates in its environment. That
is, the top-level schemas tend to be goals" (D'Andrade 1992:30). Since many of
these high-level schemas are cultural models, culture sets goals and therefore
has motivational force.
Cultural models are also internalized in disparate ways by different
actors (Shore 1996). For some people, a given model may be regarded with
relative indifference, while for others it may be highly salient. In each case,
the motivational significance of the model will vary according to the degree to
which the model is internalized and the degree of affective force it carries
(D'Andrade 1992). To determine the motivational salience of any cultural model
for a given individual, it is necessary to understand something about the extent
to which the model has acquired emotional force during her or his life history.
However, some extremely high-level models will seem more "natural" and thus tend
to motivate the behavior of most people in a given society (e.g., "individual
rights" and "equality" in the United States).
Problem #3 - Contextual Variation: While CD theory has always
acknowledged the importance of taking situational factors into account, it has
lacked criteria by which to determine which cognitions are salient within a
given context (Schlenker 1992:342). Cultural models theory can provide this
guidance. In addition to varying between individuals, the significance of
schemas also differs across social contexts (Holland and Quinn 1987). Thus,
while a restaurant script mediates behavior when people dine out, few follow
this event sequence when eating at home. Such alternative schemas may even be
inconsistent. To determine which cognitive schema are germane in such
situations, a researcher must specify the contextual background that frames an
interactional sequence using discourse analysis, life-histories, behavioral
observations, and/or the study of socialization practices (D'Andrade and Strauss
1992; Holland and Quinn 1987).
Problem #4 - The Self: CD researchers have found that dissonance
is strongest in situations which necessitate behavior that violates a person's
self-concept. Aronson states that this psychological discomfort occurs because
people strive to "preserve a consistent, stable, predictable . . . competent . .
. morally good sense of self" (1992:305). Anthropologists have pointed out,
however, that this more extreme view of the egocentric self ignores the fact
that the self-concept may vary across social contexts.
Katherine Ewing (1990), for example, has proposed a "model of shifting
selves" in which people are conceptualized as having multiple, often
inconsistent self-concepts that, while experienced as "whole" at a given moment
in time, are contextually defined. To illustrate her point, Ewing describes how
one of her Pakistani informants, Shamim, held disparate representations of
herself (i.e., as a "good daughter" who would obediently follow her parents'
wishes and become a good wife and as a "politician" who employed various
strategies to achieve her personal educational/work goals) in different
contexts. We can use this anthropological notion of the contextually-defined
self-concept to refine CD theory. In particular, as opposed to arising when a
transcendent concept of the self is violated, dissonance can be said to occur
when a person must act in ways that violate a contextually-defined
self-representation.
Problem #5 - Emotion: Festinger's model of cognitive dissonance is
predicated on the Western Judeo-Christian emphasis on conceptual consistency.
Anthropologists have shown, however, that in other cultures dissonance often
arises only in situations in which discordant cognitions are emotionally salient
to actors (DeVos 1975; Nuckolls
1993). The notion that dissonance occurs only in situations which necessitate
behavior that violates a person's contextually-defined self-representation
dovetails nicely with the anthropological view that emotions are "embodied
thoughts, thoughts steeped in the apprehension that 'I' am involved" (Rosaldo
1984:143). In other words, situations that implicate the self are emotionally
significant.
George DeVos has pointed out that, in most societies, people are
socialized by taboos to avoid directing aggression toward those with whom they
share a collective identity and social bonds: "We cannot tolerate the emotional
conflict, or as I term it the 'affective dissonance' which results from killing
within the social group" (1975:82). As we shall see, one way of getting
individuals to kill in such a situation is to redefine who belongs to the group
so that the victims are excluded from the ingroup and dehumanized as the
"enemy." What is important to note, however, is that when a person must act in a
manner that violates a contextually-defined self-concept (e.g., to kill a person
with whom she or he shares a social identity), she or he will likely experience
an emotionally charged form of cognitive dissonance. This insight forms the
basis for a model of psychosocial dissonance.
Anthropological Contributions: A Model of Psychosocial
Dissonance
Drawing on the aforementioned anthropological insights, we can now
reformulate Aronson's theory of cognitive dissonance in the following manner.
Cognitive dissonance arises when an (often culturally informed)
emotionally salient cognition about the (culturally informed and
context-dependent) self comes into conflict with another (often culturally
informed) emotionally salient cognition that motivates behavior which violates
that context-dependent self-concept. Psychosocial dissonance is reserved
for those cases in which an emotionally salient cultural model about the
context-dependent self comes into conflict with another emotionally salient
cultural model that violates that context-dependent self-concept. Thus, Shamim
experienced PSD when her parent's demands that she marry (i.e., the "good
daughter" model that shaped her self-concept in familial interactions) began to
conflict with her desire to achieve personal occupational goals (i.e., the
"clever politician" model that informed her self-representations in the
workplace). Whereas previously these contradictory models had been salient in
different contexts, they began to overlap and caused PSD. PSD is thus a subset
of CD, since all cultural models are cognitions, but not vice versa.
The degree of dissonance is a function of the emotional salience of a
cognition. If being a "good daughter" was not particularly important to Shamim,
she would only have experienced a small degree of PSD when her parents raised
the subject of marriage. Because both the "good daughter" and the "clever
politician" models were extremely important to her, however, Shamim experienced
a great deal of PSD. People are motivated to reduce such dissonance because the
cognitions in question establish goals that necessitate contradictory courses of
action. In Shamim's case, her goal of getting married established a behavioral
sequence that directly conflicted with her goal of advancing her career. I will
examine the precise ways in which people attempt to reduce such dissonance in a
later section.
First, I will illustrate how this model of Psychosocial Dissonance can be
applied to genocidal acts, using Cambodia as an example. As DeVos notes, in
almost every society, powerful injunctions exist against killing other members
of a social community. Within some contexts, however, the destruction of
"others" is often culturally and/or legally legitimated (e.g., military, penal
code, political enemies). Because cultural norms against killing tend to be so
strong, some of the individuals who kill will experience PSD. In the case of
large-scale genocide, PSD has the potential to be much greater because these
acts are often being perpetrated against people who were formerly members of the
same society. In other words, two extremely salient models fostering extremely
contradictory behavioral goals -- "kill" versus "don't kill" -- begin to overlap
within a context, thus potentially causing a great deal of PSD for the "agents
of death." If a regime is to accomplish its genocidal goals, it must make
changes in the environment that will potentially decrease the degree of PSD its
minions experience. These perpetrators of genocide will draw both on this "state
level" response and on their own "individual level" dissonance reduction
strategies to achieve consonance with the idea of killing. This is exactly what
transpired in Cambodia.
Psychosocial Dissonance Applied: the Cambodian Genocide
Many people have been struck by both the friendly demeanor of Cambodians
and their ostensible lack of conflict in daily interactions. Given the
harmonious atmosphere that is so prominent in everyday life, it is easy to be
somewhat taken aback by the political violence that has characterized Cambodian
history. Such violence illustrates the fact that while everyday communal life
(i.e., relations with fellow members of a family, village, or organization) is
frequently mediated by a high-level cultural model that fosters prosocial
behavior, larger sociopolitical interactions (i.e., relations with an "enemy" in
military activity, law enforcement, or national politics) are often informed by
an extremely salient, yet potentially contradictory cultural model that promotes
aggression. These two models, which I will hereafter respectively refer to as
the "gentle ethic" and the "violent ethic," were significant in different
interactional contexts and thus rarely came into conflict in pre-DK Cambodia.[iii] The conditions for PSD
arose, however, when the violent ethic was legitimated in everyday communal
interactions during DK. The unfortunate result was a situation in which acts of
extraordinary violence took place.
The Gentle Ethic
While disputes sometimes occurred, the few ethnographies that were
conducted in pre-DK Cambodia are striking in their description of the overall
harmony of village life (Delvert 1961; Ebihara 1968; Martel 1975). Over the
course of a year, for example, May Ebihara observed only a few significant
intracommunal quarrels. This apparent lack of discord was due to several
factors. Members of a community often shared a strong sense of social solidarity
that was developed through years of association, cooperative labor exchanges,
mutual aid, overlapping friendship and kinship networks, communal activities,
and a sense of identification with and loyalty to the group (Ebihara 1968).
Cambodian socialization practices emphasized that, when interacting
within such a known community, a person should attempt to "have friendly
relations with others" (roap an knea, reak teak). In addition to avoiding
conflict and potentially making patronage connections, Cambodians who were
friendly and polite were respected by others and thus gained honor (Hinton
n.d.). Individuals who transgressed social norms, in contrast, were subject to
gossip, avoidance, and public censure. In a culture in which "face"
(mukh) is highly valued, the threat of such potentially shameful
(khmas ke) consequences represented an effective control on individual
behavior.
Buddhism also promoted prosocial behavior among villagers. The temple,
for example, reinforced social bonds by serving as a center both for communal
and religious activity and for the moral education for children (Ebihara 1968;
Martel 1975). While people did not necessarily know the intricacies of Buddhist
doctrine, all were familiar with the five moral precepts which told them not to
lie, steal, have immoral sexual relations, drink, or kill living creatures. Such
prohibitions were coupled with Buddhist notions of karma and merit that
encouraged villagers to maintain harmonious relations (Ebihara 1968).
Although domestic violence existed, intrafamilial relations, like
communal life, appear to have been generally harmonious. Family members were
tied together by economic production, daily social interaction, sharing, and
joint participation in important ceremonies (Ebihara 1968; Martel 1975). Harmony
was also promoted by rules of proper etiquette (Ebihara 1968; Ledgerwood 1990).
Like Cambodian society in general, the family constituted a mini-hierarchy in
which people were accorded different degrees of respect depending on age and
sex. Folk-tales, didactic poems, and terms of linguistic etiquette reinforced
such patterns of appropriate behavior that could both regulate interactions and
diffuse conflict. The threat that village and/or familial spirits would cause an
innocent member of the family to become ill when siblings quarreled provided a
more proximate mechanism for curbing improper behavior (Ebihara 1968; Martel
1975). Such values and practices were part of a high-level cultural model that
fostered a disposition toward prosocial behavior (i.e., the "gentle ethic") in
the context of everyday interactions within a known community.
The Violent Ethic
Cambodia's history of violence against those defined as sociopolitical
enemies began long before DK. During the Angkorian period, "god-kings" led their
troops into battles against foreign states; both these external foes and any
internal opposition were dealt with in a brutal fashion (Chandler 1992).
Political violence was rampant during the nineteenth century and continued
during French rule. The Khmer Issarak, in particular, were known for a style of
ruthless violence against enemies that foreshadowed later practices of the Khmer
Rouge (Bun 1973). After independence, Prince Sihanouk reestablished the royal
tradition of absolute authority (Becker 1986; Chandler 1991): opposition to his
rule was considered treasonous and dealt with accordingly. The actions of Lon
Nol's troops were not much different. Such evidence illustrates that while a
person might have been "gentle" within a known community, a different ethic
often held sway in the context of larger sociopolitical interactions.
The origins of this tradition of sociopolitical violence can be traced
back to Brahmanistic notions of status and function (Chandler 1992; Ponchaud
1977). The "naturalness" of given social roles was embodied in the concept of
dharma, the "cosmic doctrine of duty in which each sort of being in the universe
. . . has by virtue of its sort, one ethic to fulfill and a nature to express -
the two things being the same" (Geertz 1983:196). This duty could vary across
place and time. In one context, a warrior had to be respectful to his social
superiors, while, in another situation, his dharma would require him to "crush
the enemy" (kamtech khmang) without hesitation.
While the arrival of Buddhism significantly altered many aspects of
Cambodian society, this "warrior heritage" (Bit 1991) was retained and even
reinforced. By asserting that the king was the defender of dharma, for example,
Buddhism legitimated the use of force against enemies who threatened social
order (Thion 1990). This norm was reinforced by Cambodian proverbs, didactic
poems, and folk-tales that encouraged people to act in accordance with their
station (Chandler 1982; Ledgerwood 1990). From a young age, children learned
about the virtues of "warriors" (neak tâsou) who gained honor by
distinguishing themselves through bravery, fulfilling their duty, and heroically
fighting the enemy. This type of "Cambodian machismo" was premised upon an honor
code which held that those who dared to kill a sociopolitical enemy in battle
gained face, while those who did not were shamed.
One of the most popular stories was the Reamke, the Cambodian
version of the Ramayana. Set in a Brahmanical world of violence and duty,
this epic also reflects Cambodian ideals about virtuous behavior within a known
community and against a sociopolitical enemy (Chandler 1992; Jacobs 1986). Thus,
Komphâkar displays proper respect and obedience toward his elder brother and
king, Reap, while at court. On the battlefield, however, he bravely fights his
adversaries to the death in accordance with his duty as a warrior. In each
domain, a different ethic predominates. Like the followers of Sihanouk, Lon Nol,
and Pol Pot, Komphâkar was disposed to engage in acts of ruthless aggression
against those defined as sociopolitical enemies.
Psychosocial Dissonance in the DK Killing Fields
Festinger asserts a person can reduce cognitive dissonance by 1) changing
given cognitive elements; 2) changing her or his behavior; 3) circumspectly
adding new cognitions that bridge the gap between the dissonant cognitions; or
4) changing the situation in which given cognitions are salient. These
dissonance reduction strategies can also, by extension, be used to reduce
psychosocial dissonance. For example, a person experiencing PSD might 1) change
one of her or his cultural models; 2) change the behavior that one of the
cultural models entails; 3) add new, lower level schemas that bring the
dissonant cultural models into consonance; and/or 4) alter the context in which
the given cultural models are salient. This latter strategy is often difficult,
since an individual rarely has the power to single-handedly change her or his
environment. Totalitarian states, however, often do. While PSD reduction
ultimately takes place on the individual level, a totalitarian state help
transform people into "agents of death" both by: 1) promoting an ideology that
modifies existing cultural models; and 2) by changing the context in which the
given models are salient.
Psychosocial Dissonance I: The State Level
Response
When the Khmer Rouge victoriously entered Phnom Penh on April 17, 1975,
their first order of business was to evacuate Phnom Penh and the provincial
capitals (Becker 1986; Chandler 1991). This dispersal of the urban population
was designed to control the citizenry, level class distinctions, create a strong
labor base for the new agrarian, communist society, and weed out opposition.
Leading military and civilian officials from the old government were rounded up
and often executed. There was also a campaign to identify other potential
traitors (e.g., teachers, students, bureaucrats, technical workers, and
professionals). While some of these "class enemies" were killed, others were
sent to be reeducated in special camps or through rural peasant life. At least
one to two hundred thousand people died in this first wave of DK killing (Thion
1993). Having dealt with these potential sources of opposition, the Khmer Rouge
instituted a number of social and ideological reforms that served to facilitate
genocide by altering the environment in which "agents of death" perpetrated
their deeds.
DK Social Transformations: The DK regime introduced a number of
radical changes which undermined the "gentle ethic" that had previously
characterized communal interactions. Whereas Cambodian life had formerly
revolved around the village, cooperatives became the fundamental socioeconomic
unit in DK (Ebihara 1990; Jackson 1989a). Economic and ecological conditions
that had previously necessitated cooperation were rendered irrelevant. In
contrast to the polite and friendly relations Cambodians had developed through
kin/friendship networks and years of communal association, interactions between
"old people", "new people," soldiers, and local DK cadre in the cooperatives
were often characterized by fear and suspicion (May 1986; Yathay 1987). While
people had previously observed patterns of etiquette that both regulated and
diffused conflict, they were now told that everyone was equal and that obedience
was due only to "the Organization" (Ângkar).
Intergroup harmony was further eroded by the destruction of Buddhism.
Many of Cambodia's leading monks were executed immediately after the revolution,
and the rest of the religious order was eventually forced to resume a secular
life (Becker 1986; Ponchaud 1989). Temples were often physically destroyed or
desecrated, sacred texts were burned, and statuary was defaced. If a Cambodian
child had previously received her or his earliest lessons on morality at the
temple, she or he was now indoctrinated into an ideology that glorified
revolutionary violence and blood sacrifice (Jackson 1989a). Communism replaced
Buddhism as the new "religion."
Whereas the family had previously constituted the primary social unit in
Cambodian life, its bonds represented a threat to the DK regime (Ebihara 1990;
Ponchaud 1989). Consequently, the Party attempted to diminish the importance of
the family by eliminating its social and economic functions. Family members were
systematically separated by housing restrictions, relocation, communal meetings,
and long work hours in sexually segregated work teams. Such separation was part
of a larger movement to redirect familial attachment to the state (Ebihara 1990;
Ponchaud 1989). In accordance with their official policy of egalitarianism and
with their high valuation of children as the future of the revolution, the Party
subverted patterns of etiquette that had traditionally governed interactions
between family members. Indoctrination sessions informed children that they no
longer had to act deferentially toward their parents. Mothers, father, children,
and neighbors were all "comrades" (mitt) now.
DK Ideology: As the gentle ethic was being undermined, the violent
ethic was ideologically legitimated at the local level and began to inform
everyday communal relations. While the leaders of the old regime had been
eliminated and socioeconomic transformation begun, the DK regime was determined
to bring the "spirit of combative struggle" to the cooperatives. Khmer Rouge
ideology frequently employed the word tâsou ("to fight/struggle bravely")
to reference the warrior spirit. Everyone was expected to enlist in the
revolutionary fight to "build and defend" (kâsang neung karpear) the
country. The first battleground was the work site. Daily activity was
reorganized along military lines (Carney 1977; Ponchaud 1977). "Squads,"
"platoons," "companies," "battalions," and "divisions" of workers were sent to
plant and harvest crops, to clear land, and to dig irrigation dams and canals.
Like the military, this economic army was subject to strict discipline, harsh
living conditions, and long work hours.
National security constituted the second battlefield (Chandler 1991;
Jackson 1989a). In addition to feeling threatened by external enemies like
Vietnam, the DK regime feared internal foes. Everyone was enjoined to seek out
"traitors" (kbat) who could potentially sabotage and/or co-opt the
revolution. Initially, this command entailed mounting an offensive against
"class enemies" and secret agents of foreign countries who were likened to a
"sickness" that needed to be "treated" or "cut out" (Ponchaud 1977). Following
economic failures, a possible coup
attempt, increased hostilities with Vietnam, and internal fights within the
Party, a second wave of killings began at the end of 1976. In the brutal purges
that ensued, high ranking DK officials were tortured at the infamous
interrogation center at Tuol Sleng; their families and subordinates, guilty by
association, often followed. In the East Zone alone, over one hundred thousand
people having "Khmer bodies and Vietnamese minds" were killed in 1978. Such
terror was glorified "in the name of the revolution. Violence became a virtue.
Waging war became prestigious. So did smashing the enemies of the party"
(Chandler 1991:241-2).
If the battle to "build and defend" the country was to succeed, all
Cambodians had to adopt a proper revolutionary spirit. This new mentality
required both complete obedience to the DK leadership and the renunciation of
material goods, reactionary attitudes, and previous loyalties (Chandler 1991;
Jackson 1989a). Since the Party represented the people, any sign of disobedience
was tantamount to treason. As in war, such enemies of the state were subject to
summary execution. Because local officials were commanded to root out these
internal "microbes" without criteria for how to do so, a great deal of local
level variation in the pattern of violence ensued (Vickery 1984). In some areas,
hard-line cadre would execute suspected traitors without hesitation. In other
locales, officials were relatively moderate in their actions. No doubt many of
these individuals would have experienced a great deal of PSD when given orders
to kill that came into conflict with the "gentle ethic" (i.e., an emotionally
charged cultural model which prohibited them from harming fellow members of
their community) that had been such an integral part of their social identity.
We will now examine how these "agents of death" dealt with their PSD.
Psychosocial Dissonance II: The Individual Level
Response
How do people become genocidal killers? As we have seen, one factor in
this conversion process comes from a "state level response." In the case of
Cambodia, the DK regime helped to reduce PSD by altering the environment (i.e.,
by undermining the gentle ethic and bringing the violent ethic to the local
level) and by providing an ideology that could be used to modify these two
cultural models (e.g., redefining the "enemy," ordering the execution of
"traitors," promoting revolutionary violence). Ultimately, however, psychosocial
dissonance occurs and is reduced on the individual level.
This "individual level response" will vary for each person. Based on her
or his life history, an individual will need to take certain steps to become an
"agent of death." Some people may just require a suitable environment to enact
potentialities that they have already actualized. Others may have to undergo one
or a series of transformations to become a killer. Still others may refuse to
participate in a genocidal regime. Most genocidal killers probably fall into the
middle category. This section will thus be concerned with delineating several of
the cognitive "moves" such individuals may make to reduce their PSD to a point
at which they are transformed into "agents of death." In particular, I will
argue that the "genocidal self" emerges in situations in which an actor is able
to: 1) dehumanize victims; 2) employ euphemisms to mask her or his deeds; 3)
undergo moral restructuring; 4) become acclimated to killing; and/or 5) deny
responsibility for her or his actions. I will deal with each of these dissonance
reduction strategies in turn.
Dehumanization: A group of Stanford social psychologists once
initiated a mock prison experiment in which college student subjects were
randomly assigned roles as "prisoners" and "guards" (Zimbardo et al. 1974). Six
days later, the planned two-week study had to be halted after the guards
exhibited increasingly abusive behavior toward the prisoners. This experiment
illustrates two interrelated aspects of the dehumanization process that
facilitates genocidal killing -- exclusion and devaluation. "Exclusion" refers
to the process by which people lose their personal identity and are viewed in
terms of a group category that is differentiated from the larger social
community (Kelman and Hamilton 1989). "Devaluation" refers to the way in which
such groups of people are increasingly marginalized from humanity. Exclusion and
devaluation both contributed to the extreme dehumanization that took place in
DK.
In an attempt to erase hierarchical and class distinctions, the Khmer
Rouge set out to divest the populace of "individualistic" qualities associated
with a "capitalist" mentality (Becker 1986; Ngor 1987). Personal property was
abolished; work and eating were communized. Everyone was required to wear
identical black garb, to cut their hair short, to adopt stereotypical patterns
of "appropriate" speech and behavior, and to divest themselves of
individualistic traits that precluded a proper revolutionary "consciousness."
The ostensible goal was to create a homogeneous society in which the individual
was subsumed by the state.
In reality, this homogeneous mass was divided along several lines
(Chandler 1992; Stuart-Fox 1985). First, a clear distinction was made between
the "true" Khmer who were a part of Ângkar and those who were its
"enemies." Since Ângkar represented the people, any opposition to it was
treasonous. Local level cadre were ordered to root out these "class enemies" who
were attempting to subvert the Revolution. The first people to come under
suspicion were "new" people: the urbanites and rural refugees who had been
expelled from the cities and were suspect for having (in)directly supported the
Lon Nol forces which the Khmer Rouge had defeated. Their very exposure to
foreign influence and imperialism suggested that new people were not "real
Khmer" and thus enemies who should be treated in accordance with the violent
ethic. This group was sharply distinguished from both Khmer Rouge cadre and
soldiers and the "old" people who had lived under the Khmer Rouge during the
difficult war years. From the very beginning, the relocated "new" people were
"outsiders" who were treated more harshly.
In addition to being excluded from normal communal life, "new" people and
other suspected enemies were subjected to dehumanizing practices. "New" people
spoke of being crammed into trucks for many hours during later relocations (May
1986; Moyer 1991). Often they had to defecate or urinate where they stood; the
trucks didn't stop, even if someone died of suffocation. "We were being treated
worse than cattle, the victims of methodical, institutionalized contempt . . .
we [were] no longer human beings" (Yathay 1987:73). People were also required to
work like animals. Not only were they expected to labor obediently for extremely
long hours on starvation rations, but they did so under the watchful gaze of
armed soldiers and/or supervisors who had the power to have them executed. Many
people have recounted the miserable living conditions in DK. "We were hungry,
too tired to wash or clean our clothes, and we lost all sense of hygiene. We
didn't care what we ate . . . where we had a shit, or who saw us. Disease spread
through the village - cholera, malaria, dysentery, diarrhoea and skin
infections" (May 1986:165).
These dehumanizing practices were mirrored by Khmer Rouge ideology.
People were instructed to be like oxen -- "Comrade Ox never refused to work.
Comrade Ox was obedient. Comrade Ox did not complain. Comrade Ox did not object
when his family was killed" (Yathay 1987:171). A soldier told one "new" person
that it was better that her mother had died "than a cow . . . [cows] help us a
lot and do not eat rice. They are much better than you pigs" (Moyer 1991:123).
Part of this extreme devaluation stemmed from the fact that new people were
often regarded as "war slaves" (Criddle and Mam 1987; Ngor 1987). "Many times we
heard soldiers shout, 'Prisoners of war! You are pigs. We have suffered much.
Now you are our prisoners and you must suffer'" (Moyer 1991:81). While sometimes
tolerated, such "enemies" were expected to work hard and to be obedient. If they
committed offenses, their execution would be no loss to DK. When explaining why
his commune leader, Comrade Chev, both killed and ordered the execution of so
many people, Haing Ngor notes: "We weren't quite people. We were lower forms of
life, because we were enemies. Killing us was like swatting flies, a way to get
rid of undesirables" (Ngor 1987:230).
Such Khmer Rouge were indoctrinated into an ideology which instructed
them to have no feeling for the enemy. As one cadre told me, "We were
brainwashed to cut of our heart from the enemy, to be willing to kill those who
had betrayed the revolution, even if the person was a parent, sibling, friend,
or relative. Everything we did was supposed to be for the Party." This ideology
of cutting off one's sentiment toward a now excluded and dehumanized "enemy"
helped many Khmer Rouge reduce PSD both by redefining who was to be included in
the new communist society and by creating a target group onto which they could
project any anxiety-producing feelings. Because the revolutionary struggle
continued in the cooperatives, Khmer Rouge cadre had little problem invoking the
"violent ethic" to execute these hated enemies who were threats to the
revolution, not "true" Khmer, and less than human.
Euphemism: By using euphemisms, perpetrators of violence are able
to mask the true nature of their actions with expressions that make them seem
benign or even respectable (Bandura 1986). In Nazi Germany, for example, Jews
were referred to in terms of a medicalized vocabulary that made their
elimination seem like a public health decision. Jews arriving at Auschwitz were
prepared for "special treatment" in which "disinfection squads" would pour
cyanide into their shower/gas chambers (Lanzmann 1985:155). Such linguistic
maneuvers provide genocidal perpetrators with a sanitizing "discourse in which
killing [is] no longer killing; and need not be experienced, or even perceived,
as killing" (Lifton 1986:445).
As one might expect in a culture in which indirect speech is extremely
common, a similar euphemistic discourse was prevalent in DK. As in Nazi Germany,
much of the violence was described in a medicalized manner. The elimination of
enemies was justified as a necessary "cleansing" of "diseased elements" (Criddle
and Mam 1987:164). This purification process continued throughout DK. In late
1976, Pol Pot stated: "there is a sickness in the Party . . . We cannot locate
it precisely. The illness must emerge to be examined . . . If we wait any
longer, the microbes can do real damage" (Chandler, Kiernan, and Boua 1988:183).
The race to eliminate this "infection" (Picq 1989:100) led to increasingly
violent purges both on the local level and within the upper echelons of the
party itself. In the East Zone bordering Vietnam, for example, many individuals
-- particularly the new people -- were executed because they were suspected of
being "infected" or "contaminated" by a "pro-Vietnamese virus" (Stuart-Fox
1985).
Local level DK cadres used a variety of other euphemisms to mask their
genocidal deeds. People were sometimes threatened with being taken "into the
forest," "to the field behind the village," or to "work on the mountain" (May
1986; Szymusiak 1986; Yathay 1987). In Cambodian culture, these places were
associated with the "wild," the feared non-civilized domain in which violence
took place (Chandler 1982). Before being led off to be tortured, imprisoned,
and/or executed, people were often told that they were being taken "to learn" or
"to see Angka" (e.g., Ngor 1987; Stuart-Fox 1985). Such palliative
expressions served to disguise and legitimate the acts of violence that the
Khmer Rouge cadre were committing.
Moral Justification: Both dehumanization and the use of euphemisms
make it easier for killers to morally justify their actions. In the Stanford mock prison experiment,
for example, "prisoners" constituted a category that it was legitimate to treat
in a harsh manner. Similarly, since Jews were likened to a "disease" that
threatened the larger German community, Nazis could regard their eradication as
a necessary public health decision. In each case, the group of persecuted
individuals was marked in such a way that normal moral standards no longer
applied to them.
In DK, this type of moral restructuring was facilitated by the
local-level legitimation of the violent ethic and the ideological glorification
of violence in general. As noted earlier, individuals suspected of being
"enemies" of Ângkar were strongly dehumanized and subject to execution.
Khmer Rouge cadre had been taught not only to hate such people but to destroy
them without hesitation or pity. For a person like Comrade Chev, killing
"enemies" was both a "political necessity" and a moral imperative: "If he purged
enough enemies, he satisfied his conscience. He had done his duty to Angka"
(Ngor 1987:229). As "traitors," those who criticized Ângkar's policies,
stole food, attempted to escape, conspired against the state, or did not display
the "proper" revolutionary spirit could be legitimately punished and/or executed
(Picq 1989; Szymusiak 1986). Such violence against enemies was glorified by
Khmer Rouge ideology, as illustrated by the frequent references to blood in the
national anthem, political speeches, and revolutionary songs (Becker 1986;
Jackson 1989b).
This blood imagery also invoked another theme in Cambodian culture --
kum, or a grudge that often leads to disproportionate revenge. Kum
is "a long-standing grudge leading to revenge much more damaging than the
original injury. If I hit you with my fist and you wait five years and then
shoot me in the back one dark night, that is kum . . . Cambodians know
all about kum. It is the infection that grows on our national soul" (Ngor
1987:9). Because one of the most common situations in which Cambodians developed
this type of grudge was when another person made them lose face or suffer, many
peasants harbored resentment toward the rich and powerful who looked down upon
them and enjoyed a much easier lifestyle. Khmer Rouge ideology played upon this
resentment by explaining to its supporters that the rich had traditionally
oppressed the poor and that the situation could only be changed through class
struggle (another concept that invoked the warrior tradition). When they took
power, many Khmer Rouge thus had a class grudge against "capitalists" who had
often treated them with a lack of respect, were responsible for their
impoverishment and suffering, and who had supported the Lon Nol forces that had
killed many of their compatriots.
A prescient awareness that the Khmer Rouge might attempt to take revenge
upon the urbanites after obtaining power is evident in comments made by several
survivors (Criddle and Mam 1987; May 1986; Stuart-Fox 1985). One person reported
how "Khmer Rouge speakers publicly admitted, they had been fired by
'uncontrollable hatred' for members of the 'old society'. 'We were so angry when
we came out of the forest', one speaker allegedly said, 'that we didn't want to
spare even a baby in its cradle' (Chandler, Kiernan, and Muy 1976:9). Ngor also
relates an anecdote about how, during a propaganda dance, costumed cadre would
pound their chests with clenched fists and shout over and over again at the top
of their lungs: "'BLOOD AVENGES BLOOD!' . . . Blood avenges Blood. You kill us,
we kill you. We 'new' people had been on the other side of the Khmer Rouge in
the civil war . . . Symbolically, the Khmer Rouge had just announced that they
were going to take revenge" (Ngor 1987:140-1). Khmer Rouge could morally justify
their killing as an act of revenge against people who had been responsible for
traditional class inequalities and the wartime deaths of numerous comrades.
Two other forms of moral justification for killing were also operative
during DK: the use of palliative comparisons and of torture to extract
confessions. First, the suffering and death of the people was sometimes
legitimized by negatively comparing such privations to those the Khmer Rouge
endured during the war. A common Khmer Rouge response to questions about
overwork, disease, starvation, living conditions, and/or random execution was
that "the revolutionaries suffered ten times worse than you during the war"
(Ponchaud 1977:58). Through the use of such comparisons, Khmer Rouge cadre were
able to minimize the harmful effects of the death and privations that the
populace was enduring. Second, the Khmer Rouge often sent suspected "enemies" to
prison centers where they were tortured until they confessed to their "crimes."
The conditions in these "reeducation" centers were appalling: prisoners were
often chained together in extremely hot and cramped quarters, severely beaten or
disfigured, left lying in their excrement and urine, tortured in horrific ways,
and/or randomly executed (May 1986; Ngor 1987; Stuart-Fox 1985). Such
dehumanization made it easier to torture victims. Torture produced their
confessions. Confession morally justified the entire process.
Desensitization: Upon completing a post-war study, the U.S. armed
forces were stunned to discover than only fifteen percent of trained combat
riflemen reported having fired their weapons in World War II (Dyer 1985). On the
basis of this information, basic training was revamped so that recruits would be
desensitized to killing. Indoctrination techniques were geared to getting
soldiers used to the idea of "wasting" an enemy. As one marine reported: "'Kill,
kill, kill, kill,' It was drilled into your mind so much that it seemed like
when it actually came down to it, it didn't bother you" (Dyer 1985:121).
Khmer Rouge cadre seem to have undergone an analogous desensitization
process. Recruits were put through an intensive indoctrination program that
"filled their hearts and minds with a seething, unquenchable hatred for the
[enemy]" (Sihanouk 1980:27). Propaganda meetings, ideological training,
self-criticism, and membership in various associations were all geared toward
producing this proper "revolutionary spirit" (Carney 1977). Extremely young
people from the poorest segments of society were favored for the army, since
they were, in Maoist terms, like "a blank page on which we can write anything"
(Picq 1989:60).
The Khmer Rouge had also become acclimated to violence during years of
guerrilla warfare, U.S. bombing, and civil war. Many survivors have commented on
how tough and battle-seasoned Khmer Rouge soldiers appeared when they
victoriously entered Phnom Penh. Their attitude toward violence was often
similar to that of Comrade Chev, for whom "the act of killing other human beings
was routine. Just part of the job. Not even worth a second thought" (Ngor
1987:229). One informant explained to me that at first Khmer Rouge like Chev
"wouldn't have the heart to kill people and might have even pitied their
victims. After executing a few people, however, killing became normal to them, a
way of proving their bravery." The banality of death was reinforced by DK
ideology, particularly the local-level implementation of the violent ethic and
the general glorification of blood sacrifice. Like the Nazi Doctors (Lifton
1986), some of the initiates to this world of violence reportedly had to get
drunk before they could execute their victims. Eventually, however, many of
these "soldiers were able to kill without being intoxicated. Some even learned
to relish it and bragged about it afterward" (Criddle and Mam 1987:98-9). For
such "agents of death," repeated exposure to violence gradually blunted their
sensitivity to killing.
Obedience to Authority: If many people were skeptical about
Arendt's (1964) description of Eichmann as a normal bureaucrat who was
epitomized the "banality of evil," these doubts were largely erased by Stanley
Milgram's (1974) research on "obedience to authority." Milgram found that a
large majority of his subjects would obey the experimenter's commands to
continue shocking a "learner," even when they could clearly see that the voltage
designation read: "Danger - Severe Shock." Afterwards, many subjects explained
that they were only "doing as I was told." Such research illustrates that people
will more readily commit acts of violence in situations in which they are given
orders which "must be obeyed" and can displace blame onto authority
figures.
Strong social precursors for this type of obedience existed in Cambodian
culture. Like other Southeast Asian countries, Cambodian political interactions
often took place on the basis of patron-client relationships (bâks puok,
khsae royeah). In return for protection and assistance in matters in which
only someone with more power could be effective, a client incurred a moral debt
which had to be repaid through loyalty, gifts, and obedience to her or his
patron, whose own influence was thus increased incrementally. During DK,
Ângkar became the new political authority to whom such loyalty and
absolute obedience were due. People were told not to think -- "Angkar thinks for
you" (Szymusiak 1986:151). Obedience was enforced by intimidation, spying,
criticism sessions, indoctrination, and terror.
In addition to the expectation that the populace would serve
Ângkar, soldiers were trained to unquestioningly obey Ângkar's
orders. Recruits returning from indoctrination training were reported to have
been converted into well-disciplined "fanatics who would not deviate from [a]
prescribed course of action" (Quinn 1976:24). Given that the violent ethic had
been legitimated on the local level, this military ethos of strict discipline
and obedience remained in force throughout DK. "In the civil war [such soldiers]
had been trained to kill Lon Nol forces. When they were ordered to kill 'new'
people on the front lines they obeyed automatically" (Ngor 1987:229). One Khmer
Rouge executioner told me that he killed because "I had to obey the orders of my
superior. If they ordered me to do something, I would do it. If we didn't obey, we would have been
killed."
Such obedience was reinforced by group norms. Cambodians place great
importance upon maintaining face because honor is gained through the respect and
obedience of others (Hinton n.d.). In social interactions, Cambodians remain
extremely concerned about how others evaluate the way they perform the duties
expected of someone of their status. This desire to gain honor through the
positive evaluations of others contributed to group conformity. In DK, Khmer
Rouge soldiers belonged both to Ângkar and to smaller units whose
survival often depended on one another. In addition to following military
discipline and being loyal to their leaders, individuals would also have felt
pressured to obey orders for fear of losing face in front of their comrades. One
person explained that if a soldier didn't obey an order to kill, "it meant that
she or he was a coward, the most inferior person in the group, the one who had
lost to the others." Moreover, those who distinguished themselves in the
performance of their duty gained honor and might be promoted. The result was
that "when the order came from Angkar to kill, they obeyed" (Stuart-Fox
1985:145).
The lack of contrition evinced by some of these Khmer Rouge was in part
due to their ability to deflect responsibility for the acts they were
perpetrating. Orders came from Ângkar and thus were not the
responsibility of the individuals actually committing acts of violence.
Relocation orders could be explained by saying: "This is Angka's rule, not my
rule" (Ngor 1987:195). Torturers would tell their victims to "please tell Angka
the truth" (Ngor 1987:242). Since Ângkar represented "everyone,"
accountability for such actions could be successfully diffused.
The Conversion Process: How are people like Comrade Chev converted
into genocidal killers? As noted previously, the specific inputs required to
turn someone into an "agent of death" will vary depending on that individual's
life history. One, several, or all of the PSD reduction strategies discussed
above may be pivotal in creating people who can commit acts of evil. These
people will draw upon the "state level response" as they make their own
"individual level response" to the PSD that arises when they kill. Such
cognitive restructuring involves a dialectic in which complex processes interact
to push the individual along the "continuum of destructiveness" (Staub 1989; see
also Darely 1992). The exclusion and devaluation of a group of individuals sets
them outside of a given community. Dehumanization morally justifies the harm of
these people. By using euphemisms and deflecting responsibility onto authority
figures, any remaining culpability can be diffused. As people are harmed, the
perpetrators become acclimated to violence. Desensitization makes the
dehumanization of victims seem more normal.
Many Khmer Rouge would have experienced PSD when they were asked to kill
people who had previously been members of their community (i.e., when the
violent and gentle ethics came into conflict). These "agents of death" reduced
their PSD in a number of ways. First, they changed these cultural models (i.e.,
they were able to do this because the DK regime effectively glorified violence
and undermined the gentle ethic in the creation of the "killing fields").
Second, these actors changed their behavior (i.e., once they had killed, such
violence became more routine). Third, Khmer Rouge cadre acted in an environment
that had been dramatically altered (i.e., DK ideology not only legitimated but
actually glorified the violent ethic in local level interactions). Finally,
these individuals were able to add new, lower level schemas that made the larger
cultural models more consonant (i.e., through dehumanization, the use of
euphemism, moral justification, the deflection of responsibility). The result of
this process of cognitive restructuring was the creation of "agents of deaths"
like Comrade Chev who could commit genocidal atrocities.
Conclusion: Anthropological Thoughts on Genocide
There are blows in Life so violent - I can't answer!
Blows as if from the hatred of God; as if before them,
the deep waters of everything lived through
were backed up in the soul . . . I can't answer!
Not many; but they exist . . . They open dark ravines
in the most ferocious face and in the most bull-like
back.
Perhaps they are the horses of that heathen Atilla,
or the black riders sent to us by Death.
-- Cesar Vallejo, "The Black Riders"
While I agree with Cesar Vallejo that it is difficult to comprehend how
people can come to kill each other so easily, I do think we can to understand
events like large-scale genocide. When confronted with the horror of genocide,
scholars have all too often turned away from exploring the origins of such
violence. Anthropologists have been particularly negligent in this respect.
Perhaps the relativistic ethos that guards "our" cultures of study has prevented
us from making stronger statements about the morality of genocidal events. As is
evident from this essay, I do not think that this is a responsible stance. It is
precisely because anthropologists can make insightful analyses of the conditions
that facilitate large-scale genocide that we must begin to examine this
topic.
This essay has been an attempt to initiate such a dialogue by drawing on
both psychology and anthropology. I would argue that, before discarding western
psychological concepts, we must first determine if they can be productively
applied cross-culturally -- albeit in a form modified in terms of
anthropological understanding. By doing so, we can enter into a
interdisciplinary debate about genocide. Psychosocial dissonance represents one
concept that can be fruitfully forged from a synthesis of psychological and
anthropological insights and can help us understand the genocidal events that
took place in Cambodian and, potentially, elsewhere.
This essay has not only shown the ways in which perpetrators of violence
attempt to deal with PSD, but also how a genocidal regime deliberately
manipulates cultural models to transform the consciousness of these "agents of
death." Because it can account for both microlevel and macrolevel factors, the
model of PSD I propose is able to provide a more complete explanation than can
either a "top-down" or "bottom-up"
perspective alone. For example, a macrolevel approach which assumes that
genocidal killers are homogeneous automatons who blindly follow state ideology
cannot explain variation in the "individual-level response" of disparate actors.
Likewise, a microlevel framework that focuses primarily on psychodynamic
processes has difficulty accounting for how the "state-level response" shapes
the contexts in which genocidal violence takes place. Because a model of PSD is
able to draw insights from both the micro and macro levels of analysis in a
culturally-sensitive manner, it represents a distinct anthropological
contribution to our understanding of the origins of large-scale genocide. Given
their work on violence and other complex topics, anthropologists can develop
many more insightful analyses that increase our understanding of large-scale
genocide even further. I urge them to begin doing so.
One of the most disturbing conclusions that has emerged from this
analysis is that genocide does not seem to be something only "sadistic killers"
are capable of performing. Long ago, Arendt correctly pointed out that such
killing can become banal, something almost anyone can come to do with
appropriate training and/or cognitive restructuring. If this is true, how can we
prevent genocide? First, as indicated above, we can develop analytical
constructs that provide insight into how genocide arises and, once it is
occurring, how it can be stopped. For example, the notion of PSD helps us
understand how people can be converted into "agents of death" through a
combination of ideological, sociocultural, and cognitive changes. To recognize
genocide in the making, we can therefore monitor a given society's "state level
response" to see if it is setting up the preconditions for genocidal PSD
reduction (e.g., by undermining cultural models that foster nonviolence,
promoting violence against devalued groups through ideology, producing a
euphemistic discourse of destruction). Similarly, once genocidal violence is
taking place, we can attempt to interfere with, or counteract, the "individual
level response" that converts a person into an "agent of death." Since
dehumanization is so crucial to the conversion process, we can take steps to
"rehumanize" victims. Obviously, each case must be examined separately to
determine exactly how to prevent/stop genocidal killers. While remaining
sympathetic to the spirit of Vallejo's words, we must try to produce such
sensitive analyses of genocide. We can answer.
Notes
Acknowledgments: I
would like to gratefully acknowledge the funding sources that have supported my
research: AAS SEAC Small Grant, FLAS, ISWP Dissertation Fellowship, NIH NRSA
Fellowship, NSF Graduate Fellowship, NSF Doctoral Dissertation Improvement
Grant, and an SSRC International Doctoral Research Fellowship. In addition to
expressing my appreciation to Barbara and Dennis Tedlock, Howard Stein, David
Griffith, and two other anonymous reviewers, I want to thank Nicole Cooley,
David Chandler, Bruce Knauft, Fredrik Barth, May Ebihara, and Bradd Shore for
their helpful comments on the manuscript.
References Cited
Arendt, Hanna
1964 Eichmann in Jerusalem.
New York: Viking Press.
Aronson, Elliot
1992 The Return of the Repressed: Dissonance
Theory Makes a Comeback. Psychological Inquiry 3:303-352.
Bandura, Albert
1986 Social Foundations of
Thought and Action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Becker, Elizabeth
1986 When the War as Over.
New York: Touchstone.
Bit, Seanglim
1991 The Warrior Heritage.
El Cerrito, CA: Seanglim Bit.
Bun Chân Mol
1973 Châret Khmaer. Phnom
Penh.
Carney, Timothy Michael
1977 Communist Party Power
in Kampuchea (Cambodia). Data Paper No. 106.
Ithaca: Cornell
University Southeast Asia Program.
Chandler, David P.
1982 Songs at the Edge of
the Forest: Perception and Order in Three Cambodian Texts. In Moral Order and
the
Question of Change. David K. Wyatt and Alexander Woodside, eds. Pp. 53-77. New Haven:
Yale University
Southeast Asia Studies.
1991 The Tragedy of
Cambodian History. New Haven: Yale University Press.
1992 A History of Cambodia.
Boulder: Westview Press.
Chandler, David P., Ben Kiernan, and Chanthou Boua
1988 Pol Pot Plans the
Future. Monograph Series 33. New Haven: Yale University Southeast Asia
Studies.
Chandler, David P., Kiernan, Ben, and Muy Hong Lim
1976 The Early Phases of
Liberation in Northwestern Cambodia: Conversations with Peang Sophi.
Melbourne:
Monash University Centre of Southeast Asian Studies
Working Papers.
Connor, John W.
1989 From Ghost Dance to
Death Camps: Nazi Germany as a Crisis Cult. Ethos 17(3):259-288.
Criddle, Joan D., and Teeda Butt Mam
1987 To Destroy You is No
Loss. New York: Anchor.
D'Andrade, Roy
1992 Schemas and Motivation.
In Human Motives and Cultural Models.
Roy D'Andrade and Claudia Strauss, eds.
Pp. 23-44. New York: Cambridge University Press.
1995 The Development of
Cognitive Anthropology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
D'Andrade, Roy and Claudia Strauss, eds.
1992 Human Motives and Cultural Models. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Darley, John M.
1992 Social Organization for
the Production of Evil. Psychological Inquiry 3(2):199-218.
Delvert, Jean
1961 Le Paysan Cambodgien.
Paris: Mouton.
DeVos, George A.
1975 The Dangers of Pure
Theory in Social Anthropology. Ethos 3(1):77-91.
Dyer, Gwynne
1985 War. New York: Crown
Publishers.
Ebihara, May
1968 Svay, A Khmer Village
in Cambodia. Ph.D. dissertation, Anthropology Department, Columbia
University.
1990 Revolution and
Reformulation in Kampuchean Village Culture. In The Cambodian Agony. David A. Ablin
and
Marlowe Hood, eds. Pp. 16-61.
Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
Ewing, Katherine P.
1990 The Illusion of Wholeness: Culture,
Self, and the Experience of Inconsistency. Ethos 18(3):251-279.
Festinger, Leon
1957 A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance.
Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
Geertz, Clifford
1983 Local Knowledge. New
York: Basic Books.
Goffman, Erving
1967 Interaction Ritual:
Essays on Face-to-face Behavior. New York: Pantheon.
Hinton, Alexander L.
1994 Cultural Models and the Study of
Aggression: An Examination of the Psychosocial Origins of Cambodian
Violence. Southern Anthropologist 21(2):11-25.
n.d. Why Did You Kill? Face,
Honor, and the Struggle for Power Spit in the Cambodian Genocide. Unpublished
manuscript.
Holland, Dorothy and Naomi Quinn, eds.
1987 Cultural Models in Language and Thought.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Jackson, Karl D.
1989a Cambodia 1975-1978.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
1989b The Ideology of Total
Power. In Cambodia: 1975-1978. Karl
Jackson, ed. Pp. 37-78. Princeton: Princeton
University Press.
Jacobs, Judith M.
1986 Reamker
(Ramakerti): the Cambodian Version of the Ramayana. London: The Royal
Asiatic Society.
Kelman, Herbert C., and V. Lee Hamilton
1989 Crimes of Obedience.
New Haven: Yale University Press.
Kunda, Ziva
1990 The Case for Motivated
Reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 108:480-498.
Kuper, Leo
1981 Genocide. New York:
Penguin.
Lanzmann, Claude
1985 Shoah: An Oral History
of the Holocaust. New York: Pantheon.
Ledgerwood, Judy
1990 Changing Khmer
Conceptions of Gender: Women, Stories, and the Social Order. Ph.D.
Dissertation,
Anthropology Department, Cornell University.
Lewin, Carroll McC.
1992 The Holocaust: Anthropological
Possibilities and the Dilemma of Representation. American Anthropologist
94:161-166.
1993 Negotiated Selves in the Holocaust.
Ethos 21:295-318.
Lifton, Robert Jay
1986 The Nazi Doctors. New
York: Basic Books.
Martel, Gabrielle
1975 Lovea, Village des
Environs d'Angkor. Paris: Publications d'École Française d'Extrême-Orient.
May, Someth
1986 Cambodian Witness. New
York: Random House.
Milgram, Stanley
1974 Obedience to Authority.
New York: Harper and Row.
Moyer, Nancy
1991 Escape from the Killing
Fields. Grand Rapids, MI: Zonervan Publishing House.
Nagengast, Carole
1994 Violence, Terror, and
the Crisis of the State. Annual Review of
Anthropology 23:109-36.
Ngor, Haing
1987 A Cambodian Odyssey.
New York: Warner.
Nuckolls, Charles W.
1993 The Anthropology of
Explanation. Anthropological Quarterly 66(1):1-21.
Picq, Laurence
1989 Beyond the Horizon:
Five Years with the Khmer Rouge. New York: St. Martin's.
Ponchaud,
François
1977 Cambodia Year Zero. New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
1989 Social Change in the
Vortex of Revolution. In Cambodia
1975-1978. Karl Jackson, ed. Pp. 151-177. Princeton:
Princeton University Press.
Quinn, Kenneth M.
1976 Political Change in
Wartime: The Khmer Krahom Revolution in Southern Cambodia, 1970-1974. Naval War
College Review 28(Spring):3-31.
Rosaldo, Michelle Z.
1984 Toward an Anthropology
of Self and Feeling. In Culture
Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion.
Richard
A. Shweder and Robert A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 137-157.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schanberg, Sidney H.
1975 The Enigma of Khmer
Rouge Purpose. Saturday Review August 23:29-34.
Schlenker, Barry R.
1992 Of Shape Shifters and
Theories. Psychological Inquiry 3(4): 342-344
Shank, Roger C. and Robert P. Abelson
1977 Scripts, Plans, Goals, and
Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Shiloh, Ailon
1975 Psychological
Anthropology: A Case Study in Culture Blindness? Current Anthropology 16(4):618-620.
Shore, Bradd
1996 Culture in Mind:
Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Sihanouk, Prince Norodom
1980 War & Hope. New
York: Pantheon.
Staub, Ervin
1989 The Roots of Evil. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Stein, Howard F.
1993 The Holocaust, the
Self, and the Question of Wholeness: A Response to Lewin. Ethos
21(4):485-512.
Stuart-Fox, Martin
1985 The Murderous
Revolution. Chippendale, Australia: Alternative Publishing Cooperative
Limited.
Szymusiak, Molyda
1986 The Stones Cry Out. New
York: Hill and Wang.
Thion, Serge
1990 The Pattern of
Cambodian Politics. In The Cambodian
Agony. David A.
Ablin
and Marlowe Hood, eds. Pp. 149-164. Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
1993 Genocide as a Political
Commodity. In Genocide and Democracy
in Cambodia.
Ben Kiernan,
ed. Pp.163-190. Monograph Series 41. New Haven: Yale University Southeast Asia
Studies.
Vickery, Michael
1984 Cambodia: 1975-1982.
Boston: South End Press.
Yathay, Pin
1987 Stay Alive, My Son. New
York: Touchstone.
Zimbardo, Philip G., Craig Haney, W. Curtis Banks, and
David Jaffe
1974 The Psychology of
Imprisonment: Privation, Power, and Pathology. In Doing Unto Others. Zick Rubin, ed.
Pp.
61-73. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
[i]After leading the country to independence
from the French colonial rule in 1953, Prince Sihanouk dominated the political
scene in the Kingdom of Cambodia. International events led to his downfall in
1970 when a coup took place. Lon Nol
headed the newly formed Khmer Republic until 1975 when his government was
overthrown by the Khmer Rouge, a group of Maoist-inspired communist rebels.
During the next four years (1975-1979), Democratic Kampuchea (DK) was
reorganized along strict communist lines. Increasing border tensions eventually
led the Vietnamese to invade the country and set up the People's Republic of
Kampuchea in 1979.
[ii]On anthropology and large-scale genocide, see
Connor 1989; Kuper 1981; Lewin 1992, 1993; Shiloh 1975; and Stein 1993. For a
review of anthropological approaches to violence in nongenocidal contexts, see
Nagengast 1994. See also Hinton (1994) for a discussion of Cambodian violence in
relation to debates about human aggression which, I argue, have been hampered by
"explanatory" and "descriptive" reductionism.
[iii]I am here referring to interactional contexts
(i.e., interactions that are embedded within a culture-specific framework of
background assumptions that is largely comprised of cultural models; see Goffman
1967), not spatial domains. |
|